Three generations of technology-enhanced learning
نویسنده
چکیده
The purpose of the present paper was to examine three generations of research on technology-mediated learning carried on by the present investigator’s research group. The first generation focused on examining computersupported collaborative learning from the cognitive perspective. The main focus was to examine to what extent knowledge-seeking inquiry elicited conceptual change. Problems of transferring inquiry learning culture from one country to another pushed us to examine social practices and other participatory aspects of learning that had been invisible to cognitive researchers. The second-generation research focused on analyzing patterns of participation in computer-supported collaborative learning. The emerging third generation research aims at overcoming the dichotomy between the cognitive (knowledge acquisition) perspective and socio-cultural (participation) perspective by means of long-standing and deliberate efforts of knowledge-creation, involving what is called objects of activity. Theoretical and methodological implications of the generations are discussed. Introduction The purpose of the present paper is to reflect on experiences of investigating technology-mediated learning within the frame of the Centre for Research on Networked Learning and Knowledge Building (http://www.helsinki.fi/science/ networkedlearning). For 12 years, the Centre has carried out studies related to cognitive technologies and practices of using computer-supported learning in schools, universities and workplaces. The focus has been on expert-like problem solving, a domain established by pioneer, cognitive researchers, such as Jerome Bruner and Ann L. Brown; a special focus has been on complex or ill-defined problems, which typically frustrate novices because of lack of established procedures leading to excessive cognitive processing load. Research into cognitive scaffolding (Wood, Bruner & Ross, 1976) and procedural facilitation (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987) indicated, however, that when provided with external, supporting tools and structures and real-time guidance, the British Journal of Educational Technology Vol 40 No 5 2009 879–888 doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2008.00873.x © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. novices can be helped to succeed in cognitive processes that are otherwise impossible. Hence, the central premise of the theory being developed is the socially contextualised nature of human activity: learning activity relies on socially distributed and selforganising processes in evolving networks of human actors, and supporting tools and artefacts (related to the ‘objects’ of activity, as discussed later; Engeström, 1987). Just as the capabilities of a networked computer cannot be located inside of its processing unit, human intelligence cannot be found by looking inside the human head; human intelligent activity is distributed across a ‘wireless network of intelligence’ (Clark, 2003; Donald, 1991) based on culturally and historically evolved cognitive tools and artefacts, and heterogeneous networks of people and artefacts (Hakkarainen, Lonka & Paavola, 2004). Investigators of the Centre took an active part in pioneering efforts of Canadian cognitive scientists Bereiter and Scardamalia (2003) to develop groupware systems (eg, computer-supported intentional learning environment [CSILE]) and its current version, Knowledge Forum (Scardamalia, 2002), for eliciting higher level inquiry and in-depth learning in education. Later in Finland, the present investigatordirected the cognitive and pedagogical design of Future Learning Environment, which was developed together by Medialab, the University of Arts and Design Helsinki (see Muukkonen, Lakkala & Hakkarainen, 2005). Currently, our research community aims at elaborating and applying a new ‘trialogical’ model of cognition (Paavola & Hakkarainen, 2004), particularly in respect to novelty and innovation, especially emphasising its collaborative and social matrix. The theory has direct application to both educational and work settings, and research projects examine existing practices of creating, sharing and managing knowledge, and propose theoretically driven interventions in the processes, often involving the introduction of supporting technology. The present investigator’s efforts to enhance learning with cognitive technology were deeply grounded in scientific inquiries into educational practices. Their investigations regarding technology-enhanced learning evolved through three generations of research and theoretical elaborations associated with three metaphors of learning (Paavola & Hakkarainen, 2005; Paavola, Lipponen & Hakkarainen, 2004): (1) knowledge acquisition (monological); (2) participation (dialogical); and (3) knowledgecreation (trialogical) metaphors. The traditional cognitive approach, elaborated prototypically in Descartes, examined learning as a process of individual knowledge acquisition taking place within the human mind (within-mind monologue). The human mind is considered as a container and learning a process of filling the container with knowledge (for criticism of the mind-as-a-container view, see Bereiter 2002). The participation or situative view, in contrast, considered learning to be a process of growing up and socialising to a social community and its norms and practices (dialogue between minds; Sfard, 1998; Anderson, Reder & Simon, 1996). The present investigator, together with his colleagues, has argued that one should additionally consider a third, distinct metaphor that addresses sustained individual and collective efforts of creating and advancing knowledge (trialogical processes mediated by shared objects of cognitive activity; Paavola & Hakkarainen, 2005). 880 British Journal of Educational Technology Vol 40 No 5 2009 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. First-generation research: the knowledge-acquisition metaphor of learning The present investigator’s research on technology-enhanced learning started while he was pursuing doctoral studies at the University of Toronto. His doctoral dissertation was entitled ‘Epistemology of Scientific Inquiry and Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning’ (Hakkarainen, 1998). The study addressed 10–11-year-old students’ research-like process of inquiry within a computer-supported collaborative learning. The issue was to examine how these students, with scaffolds, engaged in the questionand explanation-driven inquiry characteristic of scientific research. The dissertation involved a series of studies over 3 years in which he qualitatively analysed the epistemological nature of 10–11-year-old CSILE (see Scardamalia, 2002) students’ research questions and explanations. In a resulting publication, Hakkarainen (2003a) reported that a mature, progressive-inquiry culture was taking shape in Class A; only a minority of the students produced knowledge at a high explanatory level, but gradually this practice started to dominate the class. In Class B, an inquiry culture never emerged for reasons explored. Further qualitative analyses of the epistemology of CSILE students’ inquiry culture in Class A indicated that knowledge produced by the CSILE class in question was at a very high explanatory level both in Biology (Hakkarainen, 2003c) and Physics (Hakkarainen, 2004). Practically all research questions posed by the participants were explanation-seeking in nature. Moreover, the students pursued their research questions in-depth, following the pattern of interrogative activity (Hakkarainen & Sintonen, 2002) with initially very ‘big’ questions and tentative working theories, proceeding to search for answers to a series of subordinate questions (Hintikka, 1999), scaffolding within CSILE. The analyses indicated that many of the students made considerable conceptual progress (Hakkarainen, 2003c, 2004). The studies mentioned were carried out by qualitatively classifying the participants’ computer postings according to the epistemic nature of the research questions and knowledge produced. Because the students’ progress was assessed by examining their written productions, the evidence of conceptual progress was not, however, as comprehensive as one might have desired. Second-generation research: the participation metaphor of learning Back in Finland in 1994, Hakkarainen, together with several students and colleagues, started to pursue research on cognition and its support with computer technology. These studies rely on the progressive inquiry model developed in Hakkarainen’s (1998) doctoral thesis; the model guided students in advancing their conceptual ideas through computer-supported collaborative learning (Muukkonen et al, 2005). Hakkarainen’s research group, however, encountered various problems while seeking to facilitate conceptual change in Finnish science education. In some of their studies in Finnish elementary schools, they were unable to replicate the computer-supported progressiveinquiry culture prevailing in the Canadian classes despite considerable efforts involving several teachers and schools (Hakkarainen, Lipponen & Järvelä, 2002). They were not able to convey the basic concepts of inquiry to the teachers, and the computer/software focus typically led to participants becoming more interested in information and communication technology than in learning or understanding. Experiences of investigating technology-mediated learning 881 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. This finding pointed the inquirers towards participatory aspects of learning that had been invisible to many cognitive researchers. The present investigator gradually realised that learning takes place within communities of practice (Lave & Wenger, 1991), which guide and constrain the participants’ activities in multiple ways. The investigator also drew upon Vygotsky’s (1978) view that learning processes are first manifested in external (extraindividual) form. The students’ inquiry process, diffused over several collaborating individuals, took on a new character, as students scaffolded one another. This finding illustrated the crucial requirement to transform the social practices of learning that prevail in any given context. To better understand these processes, the present investigator and his colleagues teamed up with innovative teachers, started videotaping classroom practices and learned social network analysis (SNA; Hakkarainen & Palonen, 2003; Lipponen, Veermans, Lallimo & Hakkarainen, 2003). This method allowed them to examine patterns of participation and inquire into the ‘social infrastructures’ (Bielaczyc, 2006) needed to make computer-supported learning work. Simultaneously, the present investigator was asked to investigate, on a national basis, processes of innovation in Finland: how various working communities innovated in their respective subject areas. He investigated how experts’ tacit knowledge and knowledge practices are conceptualised for communal use and, vice versa, how collective knowledge is transformed to new knowledge practices (Palonen, Hakkarainen, Talvitie & Lehtinen, 2004). Methods and practices of networked expertise prevailing in knowledge-intensive business organisations were examined with a view to helping educational and training systems to answer the new challenges of a knowledge society. Networked expertise develops interactively through blending competencies across several domains of knowledge (Hakkarainen, Palonen, Paavola & Lehtinen, 2004). SNA was used to identify key actors, structures and practices of expert communities, and their dynamically developing shared objects of activity. Cultural psychology (Cole, 1996) and activity theory (Engeström, 1987) started to emerge as approaches that provided conceptual tools that allowed us to understand, theoretically, the complex relational processes involved. Third-generation research: the knowledge-creation metaphor of learning Something was, however, still missing, and the dynamics of learning were only partially grasped. My group was studying social networks and practices, but are losing a relation to epistemic processes essential for in-depth understanding as well as advancement of knowledge. Many versions of participatory and situational approaches on cognition appeared to reduce learning and intelligence to shared practices and social structures (Hakkarainen, 2003b). The present investigator found that these approaches did not provide a sufficient basis for understanding the actual epistemic processes of the turbulent, emerging knowledge society. At present, learning is neither a mere assimilation of existing knowledge nor growing up through apprenticeship in a stable community. Persons are called upon to meet novel challenges and to engage in systematic creative reinvention of their inquiry practices so as to elicit knowledge processes characterised by novelty and conceptual innovation (Knorr-Cetina, 1999, 2001). 882 British Journal of Educational Technology Vol 40 No 5 2009 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. The present investigator and his colleagues found such successful settings in certain research establishments and designated them as innovative knowledge communities (IKCs) rather than traditional communities of practice (Hakkarainen, Paavola & Lipponen, 2004). So the present investigator’s group began to compare the most promising models of IKCs, ie, Carl Bereiter’s (2002) version of the knowledge-building approach, Nonaka and Takeuchi’s (1995) knowledge-creation model for companies, and Engeström’s (1987, 1999) expansive-learning approach (Paavola et al, 2004). Within these communities, I observed social practices and knowledge practices tailored to promoting continuous innovation and change (Knorr-Cetina, 1999, 2001). Similarly with individual experts (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1993), IKCs pursue challenging projects by relying on limited resources of fluid intelligence; they develop new practices and procedures that transform novel activities into routines (crystallised intelligence), thereby releasing new resources for attaining even more challenging project goals. These investigations indicated the need to articulate a more general theory of knowledge creation, as well as to anchor the progressive-inquiry model more closely in social practices. Currently, the present investigator and his colleagues are struggling to understand learning as a form of trialogical activity focused on collaboratively advancing a shared object of inquiry, whether it is a research problem, theory, plan, product, practice (to be transformed) or project. The concept of ‘object’ has philosophic roots in Hegel’s and Marx’s as well as Peirce’s and Popper’s studies, and psychological roots in activity theory as developed by Vygotsky (1978) and elaborated by Engeström (1987) and Engeström and Blackler (2005). The object arises, within Engeström’s perspective on human activity, as essentially signand tool-mediated (see also Skagestad, 1993). Semiotic interpretation of Popper’s (1972) notion of objective knowledge comes very close to the present approach. The present investigator draws from Popper’s philosophic vision of three worlds: ‘one day we will have to revolutionize psychology by looking at the human mind as an organ for interacting with the objects of the third world [World 3]; for understanding them, contributing to them, participating in them; and for bringing them to bear on the first world [World 1]’ (p. 156). Popper examined human cognitive evolution in terms of such objects: ‘Human evolution proceeds, largely, by developing new organs outside of our bodies or persons, “exosomatically”, as biologists call it, or “extrapersonally”. These new organs are tools, weapons, machines, or houses’ Popper (1972, p. 238). Investigators examining scientific knowledge creation are concerned about ‘epistemic objects’ existing at the edge of their epistemic horizon and incorporating what the investigators do not yet know (Rheinberger, 1997; see also Miettinen & Virkkunen, 2005). The challenge is to develop methods of empirical, psychological inquiry into these ‘objects’ (an early example, Bereiter, 2002). Our model of learning holds the promise of more general applicability in the investigation of the mediated nature of human activity. The ‘trialogical’ account of inquiry, from which I draw, helps us to understand signand tool-mediated activities (Engeström, 1987; Skagestad, 1993; Vygotsky, 1978). Moreover, the trialogical approach being developed appears to provide a unified framework for research related to flow (merged Experiences of investigating technology-mediated learning 883 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. intellectual and socio-emotional efforts; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996), personal projects (individual or interpersonal endeavours that structure human activity; Little, 1993; Salmela-Aro & Nurmi, 1997) and scientific creativity (pursuit of a supportive and dynamically developing network of enterprises; Gruber, 1981). All of these are objectoriented processes integrating the epistemic, socio-emotional and social aspects of learning. Here, dialogue with various cultural–psychological approaches, such as knowledge building, activity theory, distributed cognition, actor-network theory or situated cognition, will provide valuable guidance. Knowledge-creating learning can be seen as a self-organising system (Resnick, 1994) that involves coevolution of agents, artefacts, and dynamically evolving social communities and networks (Engeström, 1999; Hakkarainen et al, 2004; Tuomi, 2002). Thus, the issues discussed are not only academic, but also relate to the challenge of European education to devise ways of preparing learners to engage in intensive work focused on deliberate knowledge advancement (Hakkarainen et al, 2004) through evolving forms of collaborative teamwork and sustained knowledge sharing and creative advance. The Centre is coordinating the Knowledge Practices Laboratory project (KP-Lab; see http://www.kp-lab.org); this integrated European Community project is focused on developing ‘trialogical technologies’ aimed at facilitating innovative practices of working with knowledge (‘knowledge practices’) in schools and workplaces (Hakkarainen et al, 2006). The intention is to refine the cognitive–theoretical principles underlying what the present investigator and his colleagues call trialogical technologies, ie, semantic-web-based tools which support long-standing and extended work in advancing and developing shared knowledge objects. The project aims at eliciting such creative practices of working with knowledge that characterise the work of experts in rapidly changing knowledge-intensive organisations and networks. The project involves extensive longitudinal investigation of transforming knowledge practices in universities, polytechnics and professional organisations. Students of higher education (universities and polytechnics) solve complex problems for real customers (enterprises, public organisations and research communities) and engage, early on, in field studies aimed at gaining experience of the professional knowledge practices in their own field; simultaneously, professional communities encounter new ideas and practices from the students, so as to bring about cross-fertilising knowledge practices between educational and professional communities. The project includes extensive research training and the participation of tens of professors and hundreds of months of graduate and doctoral students’ work representing 22 organizations from 14 European countries. Concluding remarks The present investigator’s research community examines various individual and collective problem solving and knowledge advancement processes taking place in educational and professional domains. The ‘trialogical’ research approach being developed does not address social and cultural processes in general; its special focus is on epistemic processes given form by the matrix of social processes and structures. It is focused on examining reciprocal individual, social and cognitive–technological transformations taking place in such processes. It appears essential to examine human cognition devel884 British Journal of Educational Technology Vol 40 No 5 2009 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. opmentally and longitudinally in order to reveal self-organising and ‘transactive processes’ (Samerroff & Mackenzie, 2003) of cognitive growth based on reciprocally interacting individual and social processes and their objects, within a given cultural environment. Such processes imply that the objects created and practices enacted change the environment of participants’ activity in a way that makes pursuit of more demanding objects accessible for subsequent activity. These investigations aim at creating a comprehensive view of human cognition, addressing sustained individual and collective processes of deliberate knowledge advancement, considering the conceptual, physically embodied, and practice-related aspects of such processes. Along with theoretical and conceptual development, one needs corresponding improvements of research method in order to expand investigations to knowledge practices involved in trialogical processes. The problem of accessing and characterising the ‘object’ necessitated that the present investigator use multiple methods of cognitive research, such as participant observations, structured interviews, thinkingaloud protocols and validated self-report instruments. The computer environment is designed to provide affordances for objects; the ‘portfolios’ or folders offered by the software have allowed us to examine, at all stages of their elaboration, sketches, photos and plans posted to the common database. For the recorded verbal productions, the present investigator has employed various methods of frequency analysis, multivariate statistics and social network analysis. Several investigations were design experiments (Collins, Joseph & Bielaczyc, 2004) involving cycles of (1) developing technologyenhanced learning environments; (2) implementing these technologies in educational practices; and (3) collecting empirical data that guided further theoretical and technological development. Trialogical learning processes, at best, take place across long periods of time and involve profound cognitive transformations that reshape the human mind and brain. Because some of such temporally extended cognitive processes cannot be easily experimentally studied, the present investigators are engaged in developing rigorous methods of collecting longitudinal data of complex interactive processes of learning and cognition. Within the frames of KP-Lab project, research instruments have been developed based on 3G mobile devices for doing contextual activity sampling (Muukkonen, Hakkarainen, Inkinen, Lonka & Salmela-Aro, 2008), based on ecological momentary assessment (Reis & Gable, 2000) and experience sampling method (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996). Thus, we are collecting time-series data regarding transformations of trialogical objects worked upon as well as longitudinal changes in knowledge practices of studentsof universities and polytechnics. ReferencesAnderson, J. R., Reder, L. M. & Simon, H. A. (1996). Situated learning and education. EducationalResearcher, 25, 4, 5–11.Bereiter, C. (2002). Education and mind in the knowledge age. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Experiences of investigating technology-mediated learning 885 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1993). Surpassing ourselves. An inquiry into the nature and impli-cations of expertise. Chicago: Open Court.Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (2003). Learning to work creatively with knowledge. In E. DeCorte, L. Verschaffel, N. Entwistle & J. van Merriënboer (Eds), Powerful learning environments:Unraveling basic components and dimensions (pp. 55–68). (Advances in Learning and InstructionSeries). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science.Bielaczyc, K. (2006). Designing social infrastructure: critical issues in creating learning environ-ments with technology. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15, 301–329.Clark, A. (2003). Natural-born cyborgs: minds, technologies, and the future of human intelligence.Oxford: Oxford University Press.Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-sity Press.Collins, A., Joseph, D. & Bielaczyc, K. (2004). Design research: theoretical and methodologicalissues. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13, 15–42.Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. NewYork: Harper Perennial.Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit.Engeström, Y. (1999). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Y. Engeström,R. Miettinen & R.-L. Punamäki (Eds), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 19–38). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Engeström, Y. & Blackler, F. (2005). On the life of object. Organization, 12, 307–330.Gruber, H. (1981). Darwin on man: a psychological study of scientific creativity (2nd ed.). Chicago:The Chicago University Press.Hakkarainen, K. (1998) Epistemology of inquiry and computer-supported collaborative learning(Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto).Hakkarainen, K. (2003a). Emergence of progressive-inquiry culture in computer-supportedcollaborative learning. Learning Environments Research, 6, 199–220.Hakkarainen, K. (2003b). Can cognitive explanations be eliminated? Science and Education, 12,671–689.Hakkarainen, K. (2003c). Progressive inquiry in computer-supported biology classroom. Journalof Research in Science Teaching, 40, 10, 1072–1088.Hakkarainen, K. (2004). Pursuit of explanation within a computer-supported classroom.International Journal of Science Education, 24, 979–996.Hakkarainen, K. & Palonen, T. (2003). Patterns of female and male students’ participationin peer interaction in computer-supported learning. Computer and Education, 40, 4, 327–342.Hakkarainen, K. & Sintonen, M. (2002). Interrogative model of inquiry and computer-supportedcollaborative learning. Science and Education, 11, 25–43.Hakkarainen, K., Lipponen, L. & Järvelä, S. (2002). Epistemology of inquiry and computer-supported collaborative learning. In T. Koschmann, N. Miyake & R. Hall (Eds), CSCL2: carryingforward the conversation (pp. 129–156). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Hakkarainen, K., Lonka, K. & Paavola, S. (2004). Networked intelligence: how artifacts and commu-nities expand intellectualresources. A paper presented at the Scandinavian Summer Cruise at theBaltic Sea (theme: Motivation, learning and knowledge building in the 21st Century), Orga-nized by Karoliniska Institutet, EARLI SIG Higher Education and IKIT, June 18–21, 2004.http://www.lime.ki.se/uploads/images/517/Hakkarainen_Lonka_Paavola.pdfHakkarainen, K., Paavola, S. & Lipponen, L. (2004). From communities of practice to innovativeknowledge communities. LLine–Lifelong Learning in Europe, 9, February 2004, 74–83.Hakkarainen, K., Palonen, T., Paavola, S. & Lehtinen, E. (2004). Communities of networkedexpertise: professional and educational perspectives. Advances in Learning and Instruction Series.Amsterdam: Elsevier.886 British Journal of Educational TechnologyVol 40 No 5 2009 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. Hakkarainen, K., Ilomäki, L., Paavola, S., Muukkonen, H., Toiviainen, H., Markkanen, H., et al(2006). Design principles and practices for the knowledge practices laboratory (KP-Lab)project. In W. Nejdl & K. Tochtermann (Eds), Innovative approaches for learning and knowledgesharing proceedings of the first European conference on technology-enhanced learning, EC-TEL lecturenotes in Computer Science (pp. 603–608). Berlin, Germany: Springer.Hintikka, J. (1999). Inquiry as inquiry: a logic of scientific discovery. Selected papers of JaakkoHintikka, Vol. 5. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Knorr-Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: how the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge, MA:Harvard University Press.Knorr-Cetina, K. (2001). Objectual practice. In T. Schatzki, K. Knorr-Cetina & E. von Savigny(Eds), The practice turn in contemporary theory (pp. 175–188). London: Routledge.Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991) Situated learning: legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Lipponen, L., Veermans, M., Lallimo, M. J. & Hakkarainen, K. (2003). Patterns of participationand discourse in elementary students’ computer-supported collaborative learning. Learningand Instruction, 13, 487–509.Little, B. (1993). Personal projects and the distributed self: Aspects of a conative psychology.In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self. Volume 4: The self in social perspective(pp. 157–185). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Miettinen, R. & Virkkunen, J. (2005). Epistemic objects, artifacts and organizational change.Organization, 12, 437–456.Muukkonen, H., Lakkala, M. & Hakkarainen, K. (2005). Technology-mediation and tutoring: howdo they shape progressive inquiry discourse? Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 4, 527–565.Muukkonen, H., Hakkarainen, K., Inkinen, M., Lonka, K. & Salmela-Aro, K. (2008).CASS-methods and tools for investigating higher education knowledge practices. In G. Kanse-laar, V. Jonker, P. A. Kirschner & F. J. Prins (Eds), International Perspectives in the Learning Sciences:Creating a Learning World: Proceedings of the Eight International Conference for the Learning Sciences– ICLS 2008, Volume 3. International Society of the Learning Sciences, Inc.Nonaka, I. & Takeuchi, H. (1995). The knowledge-creating company: how Japanese companies createthe dynamics of innovation. New York: Oxford University Press.Paavola, S. & Hakkarainen, K. (2004). ‘Trialogical’ processes of mediation through conceptual arte-facts. A paper presented at the Scandinavian Summer Cruise at the Baltic Sea (theme: Motiva-tion, learning and knowledge building in the 21st Century), (Organized by KaroliniskaInstitutet, EARLI SIG Higher Education, and IKIT). Retrieved June 18–21, 2004, from http://www.lime.ki.se/uploads/images/537/Baltic2004_Paavola_Hakkarainen.pdfPaavola, S. & Hakkarainen, K. (2005). The knowledge creation metaphor—an emergent episte-mological approach to learning. Science and Education, 14, 537–557.Paavola, S., Lipponen, L. & Hakkarainen, K. (2004). Modelling innovative knowledge communi-ties: a knowledge-creation approach to learning. Review of Educational Research, 74, 557–576.Palonen, T., Hakkarainen, K., Talvitie, J. & Lehtinen, E. (2004). Network ties, cognitive centrality,and team interaction within a telecommunication company. In H. Gruber, E. Boshuizen & R.Bromme (Eds), Professional learning: Gaps and transitions on the way from novice to expert (pp.273–294). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.Popper, K. (1972). Objective knowledge: an evolutionary approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Reis, H. T. & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event sampling and other methods for studying daily experience.In H. T. Reis & C. Judd (Eds), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.190–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.Resnick, M. (1994). Turtles, termites, and traffic jams. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Rheinberger, H. J. (1997). Toward a history of epistemic things: synthesizing proteins in the test tube.Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Salmela-Aro, K. & Nurmi, J.-E. (1997). Goal contents, well-being and life context during transi-tion to university: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20,471–491.Experiences of investigating technology-mediated learning 887 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency. Samerroff, A. J. & Mackenzie, M. (2003). Research strategies for capturing transactional modelsof development: the limit of the possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 613–640.Scardamalia, M. (2002). Knowledge forum (advanced beyond CSILE). Journal of DistanceEducation, 17, 23–28.Sfard, A. (1998). On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one. Educa-tional Researcher, 27, 4–13.Skagestad, P. (1993). Thinking with machines: intelligence augmentation, evolutionary episte-mology, and semiotic. The Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems, 16, 2, 157–180.Tuomi, I. (2002). Networks of innovation: change and meaning in the age of the Internet. Oxford:Oxford University Press.Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.Wood, D., Bruner, J. & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.888 British Journal of Educational TechnologyVol 40 No 5 2009 © 2008 The Author. Journal compilation © 2008 British Educational Communications and Technology Agency.
منابع مشابه
The Sociological Effects of Peer/ Teacher Technology-Enhanced Scaffolding through Process Approach on Young Male vs. Female EFL Learners’ Vocabulary Knowledge
Gender is considered a sociological construct and investigating the role of gender in foreign language learning contexts is highly important due to the effects of sociological factors in learning. Therefore, the present study set out to explore the sociological effects of peer and teacher scaffolding through the process approach in a technology-enhanced environment on the vocabulary learning of...
متن کاملPeer/Teacher Technology-Enhanced Scaffolding through Process Approach and Iranian EFL Learners’ Vocabulary Knowledge: A Probe into Self-regulation
The present study aimed at investigating the effect of peer and teacher scaffolding through a process approach in a technology-enhanced environment on vocabulary learning among high and low self-regulated learners. Participants of the study were 120 English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners who were selected based on their scores on a sample copy of the Oxford Placement Test (OPT). T...
متن کاملEnhanced Comprehensive Learning Cooperative Particle Swarm Optimization with Fuzzy Inertia Weight (ECLCFPSO-IW)
So far various methods for optimization presented and one of most popular of them are optimization algorithms based on swarm intelligence and also one of most successful of them is Particle Swarm Optimization (PSO). Prior some efforts by applying fuzzy logic for improving defects of PSO such as trapping in local optimums and early convergence has been done. Moreover to overcome the problem of i...
متن کاملThree Generations of Distance Education Pedagogy
This paper defines and examines three generations of distance education pedagogy. Unlike earlier classifications of distance education based on the technology used, this analysis focuses on the pedagogy that defines the learning experiences encapsulated in the learning design. The three generations of cognitive-behaviourist, social constructivist, and connectivist pedagogy are examined, using t...
متن کاملTiliacora triandra (Colebr.) Diels leaf extract enhances spatial learning and learning flexibility, and prevents dentate gyrus neuronal damage induced by cerebral ischemia/reperfusion injury in mice
Objective: The present study investigated the effects of a local Thai vegetable, Tiliacora triandra (Colebr.) Diels, also known as Yanang, against cerebral ischemia/reperfusion injury in mice. Materials and Methods: Thirty male ICR mice were divided into three experimental groups of BLCCAO + 10% Tween 80, BLCCAO + T. triandra 300 mg/kg, and BLCCAO + T. triandra 600 mg/kg. Cerebral ischemia/repe...
متن کاملOn Anthropomorphism in Technology-Enhanced Language Learning: Does Modality Matter in Agent-Based Multimedia Instruction on L2 Idioms?
The present study aimed to satisfy a twofold purpose: On the one hand, it sought to verify the postulation that agent-based instruction could offer a compromise approach to teaching L2 idioms where form and meaning would be equally emphasized during instruction. Given that anthropomorphism has not been much under scrutiny, this research, on the other hand, sought to ascertain whether learning a...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
- BJET
دوره 40 شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2009